This article is a DRAFT
under review for publication. Copyright 2010 by the author, Kelvin Seifert.
Cognitive
Development and the Education of Young Children
By Kelvin Seifert, University of Manitoba
As
in previous editions of this handbook, this chapter is not about cognitive
development per se, but about how
cognitive development relates to and is influenced by early childhood education.
As before, the content is therefore selective: in spite of the importance of
cognition within developmental psychology, I do not survey “all” of the
research here (even if it were possible!). Instead I select and frame material
according to its relevance to the work of early childhood educators. Since their
work usually happens in classrooms, homes, and other places with strongly
social features, the chapter also has markedly social emphasis. As a result,
some topics with strong research programs, such as the neuropsychology of
cognition, receive less emphasis than might be expected. Others, such as social
cognitive development, are discussed only for their relevance to how early
childhood educators might use the research to influence children’s cognition.
The converse of this practical idea—that educators might use children’s
cognition to regulate their social experiences—is of course important as well.
It is acknowledged from time to time in this chapter, but is more appropriately
discussed elsewhere in this volume (see, for example, Sokol, et al., 2010).
For
this particular chapter, my assumption is that early childhood classrooms are
busy, social places, and that teachers of the young normally rely on the richness
of that social life to nudge or move children toward more mature forms of
thinking. From this perspective, social experience is primarily construed as stimulus,
independent variable, or cause, and cognition as response, outcome, or effect.
Reviews based on other assumptions also exist and are in fact already plentiful
in the literature of developmental psychology (e.g. Asamen, Ellis, & Berry,
2008; Barbarin & Wasik, 2009; Haith, M. & Benson, J., 2008; Kuhn &
Siegler, 2006). The alternatives are valuable to read, and I them recommend to interested
readers. But their purposes do not speak to the needs of early childhood
educators as directly as this chapter (hopefully) does.
Fortunately,
there is ample research about the effects of children’s social development on cognitive
functioning. Most of it is founded on and continues major traditions of child
study dating back 100 years (Dick & Overton, 2010; Lewis & Carpendale,
2009). The original fathers (and mothers) of developmental psychology were very
aware that social experiences are likely to influence children’s thinking, and they
commented at length on the possible relationships. Freud described how cognitive
distortions sometimes originate in unfortunate social experiences; William
James speculated philosophically about the nature of social/cognitive links;
Piaget noted that cooperative games were both cause and effect of taking the
perspective of others; Vygotsky emphasized the importance of social mentoring
for learning common conventional knowledge. The current research has echoes of all
of these ideas.
The
chapter has several sections, followed by a conclusion. I begin by assessing
the contribution of neuropsychology (sometimes nicknamed “brain research”) for
understanding and supporting children’s cognition. As will be seen, neuropsychology
has promise for the future of developmental studies, but its current practical uses
for teachers of young children are focused somewhat narrowly, in spite of large
amounts of research in the area. The next section describes several lines of
research that looks more directly at connections between cognition and social
experience, even though in some cases the researchers did not present their
work as “social” research. First is information processing theory—traditionally
considered a thoroughly cognitive topic, even though it is in fact based on
social encounters between and among adults and children. Next are studies of
language development, in both its psycholinguistic and sociolinguistic aspects.
Finally are studies designed framed explicitly to show relationships between social
experience and cognitive development. After considering these three areas of
research, I consider again the theme of the chapter—that cognitive development
can be facilitated with appropriate social encounters—in the light of the
practical, current constraints on early childhood teachers’ work.
Neuropsychology:
Its Value and Limits
Neuropsychology
seeks to explain cognition in terms of the brain structures and functions that
animate or make cognition possible (Fusaro & Nelson, 2009). From this point
of view, cognitive development has a lot to do with biological development:
children may not be able to perform certain cognitive activities because they
have not yet developed or “grown” the necessary physical brain structures. Neuropsychologists
have pointed out, for example, that the physiological process of myelination, or formation of conductive
sheaths along nerve fibers, is still incomplete in younger preschool-age
children. Since myelination helps in conducting impulses along nerve fibers, it
is hypothesized that comparatively limited myelination may also limit younger
preschoolers’ memory abilities. By implication, efforts to teach or tutor
younger preschoolers (age three, for example) in using memory strategies cannot
be fully effective, but efforts to teach the same strategies to older children
(age eight, for example) can be more successful.
Neuropsychological
research has also highlighted a number of social
experiences that support the healthy development of brain structures (Kagan
& Herschkowitz, 2005). The most important of these happen not during the
preschool years, but during the first three years of life: nurturing of
attachment between the infant and caregiver(s), for example, very early visual exposure
to print and book reading, and a responsive, interactive language environment. Infants
benefit from these experiences because their brain physiology “expects” them;
except for individuals with certain physical disabilities, every child’s nervous
system is especially sensitive and ready for them during infancy and
toddlerhood. They can make up for deficits in these experiences later in
childhood, or even in adulthood, but remediation is not always complete, and it
is never as efficient as if children have had the experiences during the
relatively critical period of infancy.
Neuropsychological
explanations offer important general insights about why young children may be
able to perform certain cognitive skills but not others. As such it can be helpful
in grounding instruction in developmentally appropriate practices. It should be
noted, though, that the field so far has not provided advice or information for
early childhood educators that is very specific (Odom, Barbarin, & Wasik,
2010). There are two problems with “brain science” from the point of view of
early childhood educators. One is its emphasis on studying infants rather than
preschool children, and the resulting implication that the most malleable time
of life, physiologically speaking, is not early childhood but infancy. A corollary
is that interventions on behalf of infants will be more effective than
interventions on behalf of preschool children. Much of the value of the field
therefore consists of general guidance in understanding what the very young can
and cannot do—a sort of broad diagnosis of developmental capacities. Much of it
takes the form of cautions about the limits of cognition in the young: if child
X is missing brain structure Y at age Z, then teachers should not expect general
skill W to be learned at that age, no matter how well it is taught or
encouraged.
A
second problem relates to the uniqueness of its findings for teachers. Even if neuropsychology
eventually can diagnose children’s brains precisely, its value to early
childhood educators will depend on whether its findings yield insights not already
available from current, socially based knowledge of children’s development. So
far educational recommendations based on brain science coincide to a remarkable
extent with those already justified on social or cultural grounds (e.g.
Ritchie, Maxwell, & Bredekamp, 2010; Bredekamp & Copple, 1996). In this
sense the field tends simply to add support for pre-existing professional knowledge
and policy. Such support helps to build public and professional support in
early childhood education. But professional early educators in particular need
more than general support; to fulfill their mission they also need advice
related to classroom theory and practice that is fairly specific. So far
specifics are provided by neuropsychology only to a limited extent.
Information
Processing: The Interplay of Cognitive Components
Information
processing studies of development identify components of cognition, and investing-ate
how the components interact (or fail to do so) to produce children’s thinking
(see, for example, Garon, Bryson, & Smith, 2008; Hrabok & Kerns, 2010;
Marti, 2003). Much attention has gone to understanding so-called executive functions of thinking, those
associated with activity in the frontal cortex, such as reasoning, planning,
self-regulation of attention, memory strategies, and the like. The implied
research paradigm is that a younger child (or in some cases an infant, not a
preschooler) can perform a simple component X, but not a more complex component
Y without first developing another relatively simple component Z. Describing
the combined activity of X + Z, and perhaps of other components as well, therefore
provides an account of cognitive development that is more functional than
causal. Unlike accounts based in neuropsychology, the child’s thinking is not
“reduced to” or equated with biological processes.
Functional
accounts are worthy and have produced a number of findings useful to educators.
Interpreting them, however, requires keeping their intellectual risks clearly
in mind. Many information processing accounts of cognition verge on presenting only
descriptions of cognitive functioning—simply detailed observations of the
components of children’s thinking—even when they claim to present explanations
(Dick & Overton, 2010). By the standards of most educators, furthermore,
information processing research tends to simplify or even ignore the social and
cultural context in which thinking and development occurs—a tendency that Martin
& Failows (2010) call “psychologism.” Development is sometimes described
and/or explained as if it happens only “inside” the individual child, with
external influences that are minimal and under far better control than
educators normally experience. These tendencies do not render information
processing accounts unusable by early childhood educators, but they do necessitate
conceptual work in order to situate and use them in the rather complicated, social
atmosphere of classrooms or families.
A
general conclusion from information processing research on young children is that
most everyday academic skills (e.g. reading or mathematics) activate not one,
but many cognitive functions simultaneously (Fusaro & Nelson, 2009). Another
is that rendering new learning really memorable or permanent takes both
practice and time away from overt,
deliberate instruction (Bauer, 2009). Neither of these conclusions may be news
to experienced early childhood teachers, but the detailed documentation of information
processing studies can be helpful to educators nonetheless. The principle that
memory for learning takes time and repetition, for example, applies to students
of any age, but most especially to those who are the youngest. As a rule of
thumb, the younger the child, the longer it takes to insure that memory of new
material remains with the child permanently. Infants take longer than
preschoolers, and preschoolers take longer than primary-grade students. One way
that information processing theory explains the age-related improvement is in
terms of increases in the richness of children’s associative abilities.
Relative to younger ones, older children have more prior knowledge to which new
information can be connected, at least if they are shown ways to make the connections. The result is that 1) memories
consolidate (i.e. become organized and easy to retrieve) more quickly in older,
compared to younger children, and 2) there is no way for teachers to avoid dealing
this developmental difference. Older children are more “intelligent” than
younger children in the sense that they can often remember things more easily (though
this fact certainly does not make them either more worthy as human beings, nor
more deserving of teachers’ efforts!).
But
such ideas should not be taken to suggest that deliberate teaching makes no
difference. Even infants, it seems, can learn from deliberate instruction. In a
series of experiments, Bauer and her associates induced infants 16-20 months of
age to imitate an experimenter striking a miniature, table-size “gong” (Bauer,
2005, 2007). The babies showed patterns of learning very familiar to parents
and teachers of older children. As with older preschoolers, most infants could
reproduce at least some of the actions of gong-striking after just a single
demonstration, and many could still reproduce the actions even after a two-week
interval. Like older children, too, most infants could remember how to strike a
gong better if they had had more than
one demonstration of the correct actions initially. And not surprisingly, older
infants’ memories were less impaired by a two-week delay than were young
infants’ memories. Bauer interpreted these findings primarily as showing the
importance of cognitive consolidation, a relatively slow process of organizing
recent memories and linking them to prior skills and knowledge. Consolidation,
she argued, is both inevitable and necessary to form permanent memories (including
memories of how to strike a gong). But it takes significant time, repeated
exposure to the material to be learned, and slight but deliberate variation of
the exposures each time it is presented.
As
noted, these findings also describe what practicing teachers observe in
students every day. As a developmental psychologist, Bauer unfortunately does
not go on to discuss how to reconcile children’s need for consolidation with
the practical dilemmas of teaching, such as common pressures to “cover” an
overly large curriculum, insufficient instructional time, or diversity in
students’ background skills—all of which affect individuals’ needs and capacity
for consolidating material. But the findings do show relatively pure examples of
how children, even infants, can develop a cognitive skill (memory for the
striking of a gong) as the result of a social experience (the modeling of a
behavior by a “teacher-experimenter”). Bauer summarized the findings in two
slogans meant as advice for teachers: “Repeat,
with variation on the theme,” and “Link
early, link often.” To insure that learning is not forgotten, that is, teachers
need to provide material more than once, each time in a slightly different way
so that students can form new associations with the material during each
encounter. The advice is reminiscent of Jerome Bruner’s (1960) classic
recommendation to follow a “spiral curriculum,” one in which ideas are
revisited repeatedly, but each time in a new or different form.
Studies
of infant memory like these confirm the importance of links between social and
cognitive experiences for young children: it appears that the stock-in-trade of
teachers, social interaction, can stimulate cognitive development in
preschoolers, at least under some conditions. No doubt other factors, including
physical brain growth and society-wide cultural practices, also affect children’s
thinking. Since neither physical growth nor culture are normally under
teachers’ direct control, however, they are more important to teachers as
background knowledge—as reminders to hold expectations for children’s learning
that are realistic.
Directly
or indirectly, many studies in the information processing tradition have
supported the importance of talking to
and with children. They also suggest
that adults’ style of interaction matters a lot. For example, Haden and
colleagues (2001) and Ornstein, Haden, and Hedrick (2004) have studied dramatic
play “adventures” in which mothers and their 3- and 4-year-old children enacted
a make-believe scene using dramatic play materials provided by the researchers.
One adventure consisted of a make-believe camping outing; another, of a make-believe
bird watching activity; a third, of a re-enactment of an ice cream store. In
each case appropriate props were provided. The dramatic play experiences were
videotaped, and investigators analyzed the tapes for how the mothers’ interaction
styles affected their preschool children. Children were then interviewed one
day after the experience, as well as three weeks later to find out how much
they remembered. In general, the children remembered significantly more of the
dramatic play experience if the mother had encouraged the child not only to talk
jointly with her about the materials as they were used, but also to handle the props
jointly with her. Joint discussion without joint handling was somewhat less
helpful to a child’s later memory, and a lack of either joint discussion or joint
handling was least helpful of all.
In
another study using the same dramatic play events and children of similar ages,
Hedrick and colleagues (2009) analyzed the verbal interactions between mother
and child to identify the specific features of the joint-talk conversations
that might account for their effectiveness. They found that in high joint-talk
conversations, mothers more often asked open-ended Wh- questions (who, what,
where, when, why, or how), and these
tended to elicit more elaborate responses from the children. In a related program of research, furthermore, Boland and
colleagues (2003) successfully trained mothers to interact more effectively
with their preschool children. Specifically, they were trained to use 1) more wh- questions
to elicit their child’s participation, 2) more associations of the current activity to what the mothers knew that
the child already knew, 3) follow-ins,
or comments that supported and extended features of the situation in which the
child showed interest, and 4) positive
evaluations or praise for the child’s participation. Mothers learned these
techniques successfully, and children of trained mothers recalled more of the
dramatic play activities than children of untrained mothers.
A
follow-up study used a similar training procedure, but instead of training
mothers, it trained researchers unfamiliar with the children to use the same
interaction techniques (Hedrick, Haden, & Ornstein, 2009). During the later
memory interviews, in addition, the researchers were also trained to use “high
elaborative event talk,” which were interaction techniques similar to those
used during the dramatic play events themselves. The overall results gave clear
support for the benefits of the high elaborative event talk both during the
dramatic play itself and during later recall conversations. Asking open-end wh- questions led to better recall if
used during the dramatic play, as in the earlier studies, but so did asking wh- questions later, during the recall
interview or “test.” Most effective of all was to ask wh- questions on both occasions.
Put
in terms a bit more familiar to educators, the mothers and researchers using
high elaborative event talk were scaffolding
memory strategies for young children, though without actually calling the
process by this term. The wh-
questions and follow-ins stimulated classic mnemonic memory
strategies—elaboration and rehearsal—and modeled them for the child, albeit
unconsciously or subliminally. In the short-term the events created examples of
a zone of proximal development in the
Vygotskian sense: the child could do more (i.e. recall more) in the presence of
a mentoring adult than she could do alone (Hoskyn, 2010; Stetsenko &
Vianna, 2009). The long-term result was a stabilization of the child’s new
knowledge, along with a richer experience observing and using, but not thinking
consciously about, memory strategies.
There
is a further education-related inference from these ideas: it seems plausible, as
children grow a bit older, that parents, teachers, and other caregivers might wish
to transfer some of their scaffolding responsibility more fully to the child. Transferring
responsibility would presumably involve making the existence and benefits of metacognitive
memory strategies conscious within the child. This cognitive developmental goal
would seem especially appropriate for teachers of young children, since metacognitive
strategies would serve children well as they moved on into the elementary
school years.
These
ideas guided a study by Ornstein, Coffman, & Grammer (2009), who observed
first-grade teachers in action as they taught lessons in language-arts and
mathematics, and who then tested the students’ memory with a card sorting task
at intervals throughout first- and second-grade. Not surprisingly, they found variation
in how habitually and explicitly teachers described memory strategies to
students, and how often they recommended strategies or urged students to create
strategies of their own. After several months, however, the students of the “high-mnemonics”
teachers showed greater use of memory strategies, an advantage that continued
through the end of second-grade. Detailed analysis showed three further points about
the individual differences: 1) the students who benefitted the most from
high-mnemonics teaching tended to be lower-achieving students, 2) higher-achieving
students were relatively unaffected by the teacher’s use of mnemonics (perhaps
because they already were using effective memory strategies without coaching),
and 3) students of high-mnemonics teachers did not show better memory performance initially, but required several
months of such teaching to begin showing their advantage.
As
mentioned earlier, the studies just described are all framed in functional
terms: a child (or infant) can do simpler function X, but not harder function Y
without first developing at least one other simpler function Z. In Bauer’s
studies of infants remembering how to hit a gong, for example, the younger
infants were shown capable of imitating elements of the action (simpler
function X), but not of reproducing the entire sequence reliably (more complex
function Y) without first consolidating the simpler elements by associating the
elements mentally—on their own time, so to speak—in between practice sessions (simpler
function Z). In the dramatic-play recall studies by Ornstein and associates,
preschoolers could recall fragments or elements of a make-believe episode
(simpler function X), but not the entire experience (complex function Y)
without elaborating on elements of the episode while it was happening (simpler
function Z). In the studies of first-grade teachers using mnemonics, school-age
students could perform an arbitrary memory task to some extent (simpler
function X), but they did it without coaching (complex function Y) if they were
repeatedly coached not on the task itself, but on memory strategies to apply to
the task (a set of simpler functions Z).
Insights
like these highlight the relationships among cognitive skills, and they are
useful for helping early childhood educators identify and sequence reasonable
instructional and developmental goals for children. But they overlook the obviously
social elements of “cognitive” tasks as experienced by the children. Cognitive
benefits documented like the ones above develop not just because of other cognitive skills, but also because of
significant social interactions and positive emotional relationships. To state an
obvious but overlooked point: in every case a kindly, reasonable adult arranges
a particular kind of conversation or experience with the child, and the child
elects to trust the adult’s motives in the social situation thus created. The child’s
trust in turn derives from a history of social encounters that many published
accounts of child cognition put in the background because of the constraints of
experimental design. A relevant background factor might be the fact that a mother
and child (hopefully) love each other, for example, so that her child is
willing to follow an experimental procedure simply because she asks him to do
so. Another backgrounded factor might be that a teacher who recommends metacognitive
strategies to students has already developed
such good rapport with the students that they are willing to give her
recommendations a trial sight unseen, so to speak. Still other background factors
are the broad cultural regularities that influence behavior during every
research experiment, such as whether a child considers it acceptable for any
adult in authority to ask “test” questions (ones to which the adult already
knows the answer), or whether arbitrary problems framed outside of everyday contexts
are really worth thinking about.
In
spite of these comments, I should emphasize that these “social” omissions are
not the fault either of the researchers or of the information processing
framework adopted for their studies; they were not, after all, trying to
conduct social or cultural research. I mention them instead in order to place cognitive
developmental research in the context of early childhood education, a task for
which it is often not directly designed. The latter activity is primarily based
on the activities of an adult who enjoys children interacting intensely with a
number of young children or students. To be useful early childhood teachers,
therefore, strictly cognitive studies often need a degree of reframing, not because they are badly conceived or
conducted, but to take the obviously social emphasis of early childhood
education into account. The critique in this chapter is meant only for that
specific purpose.
Socially
Focused Studies of Early Cognition
Notwithstanding
the previous comments, many studies have in fact looked relatively directly at
relationships between social experiences and the development cognitive skills. These
often require less reframing than studies that are more exclusively cognitive.
How well a particular study serves the needs of early childhood teaching,
however, depends on how closely it parallels or represents significant features
of classroom teaching and learning. Since even “social” studies vary on this
dimension, it is still necessary to interpret the educational usefulness of a
developmental study carefully. In this section I attempt to make such
interpretations with regard to several programs of research connecting social
experiences to cognitive change. Most involve language in some way, though
their effects are simultaneously cognitive and social.
A Grand Tour of Social
Influences on Cognition
A
lot of cognition is acquired and influenced by the language witnessed by and
directed to a child during social activities (see Wells, 2009). The resulting cognitive
changes begin shortly after birth and continue through early childhood and
beyond. In this section I begin with a brief “grand tour” of several social
influences on cognition, and then explain the key elements of each in more
detail.
Arguably,
the first social influence on cognition is joint
attention, in which an infant and adult use gestures or vocalizations to call
each other’s attention to a commonly experienced event, such as looking at the
family cat, or commenting on the arrival or departure of a parent (Eilan, et
al., 2005). When the adult includes bits of speech to the exchange—e.g. says
“cat!” while pointing and looking at the cat—the spoken words become part of
the experience for the child (Sabbagh & Baldwin, 2005). Thanks to the
child’s initial egocentricity, and possibly also to innate human biases facilitating
language learning, a child usually assumes that speech labels are conventional. She behaves, that is, as
if the same word is used for the same object on all occasions and by all people,
and that the word is unique and distinctive from words used for other objects
or events (Kalish & Sabbagh, 2007). The assumption of conventionality simplifies
the learning of vocabulary and thus makes language development much easier. With
time and further language development, however, the child also begins noticing
variations in the certainty and reliability of other persons’ knowledge and
beliefs—clues expressed through various evidentiality
markers (Jeschull & Roeper, 2009; Matsui & Fitneva, 2007). As awareness of such variability dawns
on the child, she qualifies or limits the general assumption of conventionality
(Callanan, Siegel, & Luce, 2007). There is more than one way, she realizes,
to say the “same” thing, and individuals sometimes differ in how they refer to
the “same” object or event.
Awareness
of the variability of knowledge prompts the child to begin expressing, if not
truly reflecting on, the cognitive status of her own knowledge and reasoning—her
own degree of certainty. The rudimentary self-awareness implied by
evidentiality markers in turn allows for more effective problem solving skills
to develop; the child begins to know what she knows and what she may only
imagine or suppose. Continuing dialogues with adults supports further
improvements in the child’s problem solving skills, and features of these
dialogues are eventually appropriated by the child in the form of private speech, or self-directed talk that allows thinking and problem solving to
become more autonomous. Using private speech, the child can talk her way
through more challenging problems than in the past, and that would otherwise confuse
the child or require adult assistance (Winsler, Fernyhough, & Montero, 2009).
More or less at the same time, the child’s public speech becomes more nuanced:
she can begin understanding and using irony and sarcasm, for example, and
engage in extended dialogues at social gatherings such as meal times or in play
activities.
Early Key Social
Influences: Joint Attention and the Emergence of Private Speech
As
early as six months of age, infants show a tendency to follow an adult’s gaze
or pointing gesture, and by 18 months they have perfected this behavior to a
high degree (Butterworth, 1995). Such joint
attention—an eminently social experience—makes possible a lot of learning
because adults often pay attention to objects, people, and events that are
especially important, interesting, dangerous, or complicated. Adults often also
accompany their attention with language, which for the child’s purposes initially
becomes part of the experience (Woodward, 2003). The challenge for the
infant—one that takes months or years to accomplish—is to understand why an adult attends to or gazes at an
object. Tracing the course of the infant’s detective work is the focus of much
research about infant cognitive development (Eilan & Heal, 2005).
Both
experimental and correlational studies suggest that the language learned during
joint attention encourages a number of cognitive functions, particularly an
ability to inhibit dominant responses deliberately, in favor of less dominant
or subordinate responses. This ability is illustrated by the common children’s
game of “Simon Says.” In the game, inhibiting a motor response while also listening
carefully to verbal instructions allows a child to be more successful in the
game. In a controlled assessment of an analogous skill with inhibition, Muller
and colleagues (2009) devised what they called the “Colored Smarties” task.
Preschool children were presented with an array of colored cards, each of which
had a candy on it (the Smartie) that did not
match the color of the card. In order to win a Smartie to eat, a child had to
select a card of a particular color without
being distracted by the color of the Smartie itself. Three-year-olds had
considerable difficulty with this task, but four- and five-year-olds performed
it almost perfectly. A series of experiments with the task suggested that the challenge
for three-year-olds was language-related: if a child said the name of the card before selecting it, he or she was less
distracted by the color of the Smartie, and performed better. Older preschoolers
either used this strategy of their own accord or else discovered it for
themselves during the task. Younger children did not usually say the color words
on their own initiative, though Muller showed that they could be if trained to
do so, and that training improved their performance significantly.
The
language needed for the Colored Smarties task was a simple example of self-directed or private speech, which Vygotsky and others have argued serves as a transition
between problem solving with the assistance of linguistic support from an
adult, and fully autonomous problem solving by the child. In the research by
Muller and colleagues, for example, the child was in essence encouraged to
“talk to himself” as a way of insuring cognitive success. Other studies corroborate
the influence of language heard initially in social encounters on the development
of private speech. A longitudinal one by Landry and colleagues (2002), for
example, observed verbal interaction between mothers and children during free
play when the children were three or four years old. Two years later, when the
children six, they observed the children’s problem solving abilities. They
found significant correlations between the mothers’ earlier tendency to scaffold the child’s actions during free
play, and the child’s later skill at various problem solving tasks. Scaffolding
in this case included expressing explicit links among objects, people, and
activities in which the child was engaged. The authors argued that the parents’
language style at the earlier age provided children with language-related
models for thinking or reasoning, models which children later used without
assistance and on their own initiative.
Landry
and colleagues did not actually observe the presence or absence of self-directed
speech directly, but other research has done so. Fernyhough and Fradley (2005),
for example, used a “Tower of London” game, in which a child sorts and transfers
a stack of nested rings from one peg to another following a few simple rules.
For research purposes, the task has the advantage that its difficulty level can
be adjusted easily—there can be many rings to sort or just a few. Using this
task with five- and six-year-olds, Fernyhough and Fradley found that private
speech increased with task difficulty up to intermediate levels of challenge,
after which it decreased somewhat. The trend suggested that children used self-directed
speech to assist if necessary and helpful, but that when a problem became so
difficult that success was unlikely, they gave up using it as a cognitive
strategy.
Later Key Social
Influences: Conventionality, Evidentiaility, and Theory of Mind
When
first acquiring language, children as young as 24 months seem to make the
crucial assumption that certain features of language are conventional—that is, that people label objects, for example, everyone
uses the same word, and that a label does not change from one occasion to the
next (Sabbagh & Henderson, 2007). When taught an unfamiliar label for an
object by one speaker (e.g. “This is a teega”),
for example, a young child will later point to the same object even when asked to
identify it by a different speaker (e.g. the new person asks, “Which one is the
teega?”). Notably, the conventionality assumption is made for objects but not
for personal preferences or for proper names; apparently therefore even two-year-olds
do not assume that every person has the same preferences or the same name
(Graham, Stock, & Henderson, 2006). By age 3 and 4, furthermore, children
show a consistent preference to learn labels from speakers who express
certainty and who have a proven history of accuracy (Koenig & Harris,
2005). When interacting with two adults who label objects, one of whom has
consistently made labeling mistakes in the past, preschoolers will attend to
and remember the labels primarily from the “accurate labeler.” If a child is
asked to look for a hidden object, furthermore, the child is more likely to
follow the advice of one who says, “I saw it in this box” than one who says, “I
think it is in this box.”
It
seems therefore that preschoolers begin differentiating types of knowledge according
to its cognitive status and purposes. In particular, knowledge is distinguished
from belief, and knowledge that is certain is distinguished from knowledge that
is not certain. Skill at differentiation is developed through conversations
primarily with parents, though it is probably also supplemented somewhat by other
adults (Callahan, Siegel, & Luce, 2009). It is expressed through lexical,
pragmatic, and even grammatical features of language itself—so-called evidentiality markers (Matsui &
Fitneva, 2009). In English the markers usually take the form of words or
phrases indicating the source, reliability, or certainty of knowledge, as when
someone says, “Apparently he ate the cake,” as compared to “I saw him eat the
cake,” or “I think he may have eaten the cake.” English speakers also use
non-verbal gestures and vocalizations to indicate the status of their
knowledge—a well-timed shrug of the shoulders, for example, or a tentative tone
of voice. Many non-English languages, in addition, have “built-in” grammatical
(syntactic) features that indicate sources and levels of certainty and that may
provide additional ways to recognize types of evidence and to express the
differences. With or without grammatical features being available, though,
evidentiality markers help to develop the idea that individuals have minds of
their own: that people vary not only in what they know, but also in their
certainty and trustworthiness (Fitneva, 2009; Jaswal & Neeley, 2006).
The
conventionality assumption, it should be noted, does not begin as a “belief” in
the adult sense of the term, but more as an expression of immature egocentrism
or lack of thoughtful belief on the
part of the young child (Sabbagh & Henderson, 2007). Two-year-olds probably
do not hold a conscious principle that everyone uses language in the same way.
Instead they simply, and thoughtlessly, behave as if everyone does—and as it
happens, the assumption facilitates the learning of common language and social
relationships. The evidentiality distinctions gradually developed in these
social encounters, on the other hand, facilitate cognitive development in their
own way: by initiating glimmers of reflection about the nature and sources of
knowledge—about who knows what and why, and therefore also who has a mind of his
or her own, so to speak.
All
in all, conventionality and evidentiality contribute gradually to developing a theory of mind, or attributions about
individual mental states (Wellman, 2003). By about age four, a child comes to
act as if other persons perceive objects and events based on beliefs and knowledge
unique to each person’s experience. Prior to this age, a child is apt to
believe that others believe and know whatever the child himself believes and
knows. The phenomenon is illustrated clearly in the widely researched “false
belief task.” In one common version of the task, a child watches while an
object is hidden behind one of two boxes in the presence of a third person.
Then the object is moved to the other box while the child watches, but without
the third party seeing or knowing about the move. Older preschoolers (age 4 or
5) know that the third person will mistakenly believe that the object is still
behind the original (first) box, and that the third person can be expected to search
for it there. Younger children will instead assume that the third person knows
whatever the child himself has witnessed, i.e. that the person will therefore search
behind the second box. The ability of
the child to differentiate his own knowledge the other person’s false belief is
taken as evidence of a “theory of mind.” The child has (apparently) developed a
belief or assumption that each person experiences the world independently of
other persons, based in this case on their individual sources of knowledge.
The Cognitive
Benefits of Ambiguity: Irony
Several
researchers have argued that social interactions that are ambiguous stimulate
cognitive conflict—a sort of Piagetian disequilibrium—that motivates children
to develop their interpretive and expressive skills beyond current levels.
Ambiguous contacts may actually be relatively plentiful for young children, who
are still mastering the nuances of language as well as appropriate uses for
nonverbal gestures and social behaviors. One obvious example of such a challenge
occurs, for example, when a parent or adult uses sarcasm or verbal irony,
either directed to the child or simply witnessed by her. Sarcasm by nature
communicates double meanings; the literal words “say” one thing but the speaker
intends something else. Sorting out the two meanings requires cognitive work: the
child must hold the literal meaning of a remark in mind, but simultaneously
discern, or at least guess, a different underlying meaning or intention.
Studies show that children learn this cognitive skill by about age 5 or 6; at
about that age they know when a person is sarcastic—i. e. does not mean what he
says (Pexman, 2008, 2010). Note, though, that irony is by nature prone to
misunderstanding at any age; there is always a risk that a listener will take
the speaker literally and not notice an implied meaning distinct from the words.
Perhaps for this reason children are slower to use irony than to recognize it when
it occurs (Pexman, et al., 2009). Many do not use it at all until well into the
elementary school years, though some children do begin making ironic remarks
late in the preschool period, especially if they have parents who report using it
in their own conversations more than usual (Hala, et al., 2010; Pexman, et al.,
2009).
The Cognitive
Benefits of Ambiguity: Bilingualism
Another
sort of ambiguity that confronts some preschoolers is the learning of a second
language. Whether learning a second language or simply using one, a child in a
bilingual family has to keep two languages actively in mind at the same time, decide
repeatedly which grammar and lexicon to draw on, and inhibit responses using the
nonrelevant language. The result is continual linguistic disequilibrium that may
stimulate a number of cognitive skills that extend beyond language as such (Adescope,
et al., 2010). One such skill, for example, is cognitive flexibility—an ability
to switch attention easily from one dimension of a situation to another
dimension. Suppose both monolingual and bilingual preschoolers are given a set
of cards that vary in two ways, such as color and shape. If the children are
asked to sort the cards by one of the dimension (e.g. color), three- and
four-year-olds succeed to a large extent, whatever their language backgrounds.
If they are then asked to switch to sorting the cards by the other dimension
(shape), however, monolingual children experience significantly more trouble
than bilingual children; the monolinguals tend to perseverate on using the
first sorting dimension. Researchers have interpreted the difference as
evidence for a cognitive benefit of bilingualism: a skill at shifting attention
flexibly from one dimension of language to another, and of dealing with the
cognitive conflict resulting from the shift. The flexibility appears to
generalize beyond language as such (Bialystok & Craik, 2010).
Meal Times and
Extended Dialogue
Family
conversations at mealtimes have been studied extensively since the 1970s, and
the results generally show a number of benefits for children’s language and
cognitive development (Hamilton & Wilson, 2009; Kiser, et al., 2010). By
providing extended dialogue between parent and child, for example, mealtimes
give children opportunities to hear unusual words used in context, listen to
explanations of the words, and to begin using the words themselves (Snow &
Beals, 2006). The extended dialogue of meal times includes both extended
explanatory talk (“logical” problem solving) and extended narrative talk (story
telling), and allows children chances to enhance their own expressive skill
with these genres. The discourse skills thus acquired are important for
academic success in the elementary grades, and are also therefore important as
educational goals for preschool and primary-grade classrooms. Unfortunately,
because of issues of administration, class size, and classroom group
management, teachers of very young children are often not able to provide
extended conversations with individual children, even when their programs
literally involve serving meals (Helburn, 1995). Intervention experiments in
early childhood programs have shown, however, that teachers are quite capable
of engaging in extended dialogue with children—and indeed prefer to do so—when
supported with appropriate ideas and materials (Bradley & Reinking, 2010).
Observations of lunchtimes at child care centers, in any case, show that
preschool children engage in substantial language play, narrative, and
explanatory talk at these times with each other, even when adults are not present to participate (Holmes,
2010). From the point of view of an early childhood teacher, therefore, a
constructive teaching strategy may be to allow extended conversations simply to
happen, participate in and support them whenever possible, but recognize that
classrooms cannot always reproduce the individualized atmosphere of a family
discussion fully.
Pretense Play and
Games as Preparation for Institutional Life
Much
has been written about the cognitive functions of play (Linn, 2008; Berk, Mann,
& Ogan, 2006; Piaget, 1962). As Piaget emphasized and subsequent research
has generally confirmed, make-believe play both stimulates and uses young
children’s ability to represent objects and activities, nudging preschoolers
beyond the sensorimotor here-and-now of infancy into more logical, but still
rather concrete worlds of concrete operations. More recent writing has further argued
that that make-believe actually fosters a component of hypothetical thinking,
because children use objects “as if” they were something not immediately
visible and in this sense treat objects as abstractions—a necessary though not
sufficient feature of hypothetical thinking (Seifert, 2006). Abstract,
counterfactual thinking is a feature of formal or hypothetical thinking posited
by Piagetian psychologists for the period of adolescence, with the obvious difference
that adolescents manipulate representations that are both more abstract and
more logically coherent.
Games
with rules carry this cognitive challenge a step further than the earlier make-believe
by challenging children not just to use one object to represent another, but to
agree with others about how to represent
an object or action. Small, round pieces of wood are markers in a board game of
checkers, and they can only be moved in certain ways. But this is true only
because people who play the game agree to define and use the bits of wood in
these ways. People could instead agree to use rocks, bits of paper or glass. On
a community-wide scale—to consider a broader example—slips of paper with
certain markings on them become “money” because people conventionally agree to
treat the slips as such, even though other objects (lumps of metal or even
heads of cattle) could be defined as “money” by convention. On a culture-wide
scale, in fact, most human institutions owe their reality to objects and
actions with meanings agreed on by convention.
During
the preschool years, games with rules provide children with early experiences
of conventionally assigned statuses, and in this sense such games initiate a journey
toward understanding the nature of institutional life that characterizes much
of adulthood (Rakoczy, 2007). Young children must understand that a game of
hide-and-seek, for example, is a “game” because the players agree that it is,
and agree on the rules for playing it. The rules acquire their key properties
not because of their intrinsic features, but because of agreements among the participants.
If the conventionality is not understood, then hide-and-seek ceases to be “just
a game,” and players either fail to coordinate their actions or else hurt each
other’s feelings easily.
Later
in life, adults frequently face analogous needs to understand conventionality,
even though conventionality it may not be labeled as such: a marriage becomes a
“marriage,” for example, because everyone—the partners, relatives, and society—deems
it to be one and therefore expects the participants to follow certain rules
about marital behavior. Understanding conventionality obviously takes
experience and reflection, and misunderstandings about the status of
conventional agreements are not uncommon, either for adults (e.g. in marriage)
or for children (in games). As Piaget pointed out, individuals at first may treat
the rules of a game simply as sacred, unchangeable laws governing behavior and needing
strict enforcement (Kalish, 2005). They fail to see that rules are also
constituted from human agreement, and therefore can in principle be revised by
mutual agreement. Such misunderstanding is especially likely for young
children, who have relatively little experience with inventing, and negotiating
revisions to rules. But repeated experience at games-with-rules helps move
their views toward more mature perspectives about conventions. As it happens, early
childhood teachers are well-positioned to provide such experiences, because the
group settings that typify early childhood programs lend themselves to play and
games (Singer, Golinkoff, & Hirsh-Pasek, 2006).
How
Can Cognitive Developmental Research Be Used?
As
the preceding sections suggest, cognitive developmental research offers ideas
relevant to the teaching of young children. At a minimum the research helps early
childhood professionals to hold expectations about children’s cognition that
are appropriate, and sometimes in addition it suggests ways to intervene on
children’s behalf actively. From cognitive developmental research we know, for
example, that children can remember aspects of “instructional” experiences almost
from birth, and that no matter how young, children benefit from opportunities
to consolidate memories of experiences. Consolidation takes time, however, and short
cuts may or may not be possible. The research confirms the common sense notion
that younger children do not remember as reliably as older children, but it
also suggests that the younger children’s memory can be improved significantly when
encouraged to use deliberate mnemonic strategies.
There
is also ample evidence that children benefit cognitively from extended
conversations in a variety of situations—eating meals, reading story books, interacting
with a second language, playing at make-believe or games-with-rules. Cognitive
developmental research also suggests why extended conversation helps: it exposes
the child to new vocabulary and gives chances to practice using complex
discourse genres. Conversation also engages the child’s attention jointly with
adults’. The joint attention makes it more likely that a child will learn not
only what adults consider to be important, but also ways to talk that recognize
universal conventions of meaning as well as diversity in the credibility and
reliability of others’ knowledge. Problem solving becomes more clear-headed and
focused as a result.
Yet
if history is a guide, ideas such as these will make their way into professional
practice only partially and slowly. There are several reasons for this
assertion, some of which are easier to deal with than others. One reason is
that teachers’ sometimes just lack knowledge of recent relevant research. An
interview study by Deniz (2009), for example, found that many early childhood
teachers’ were not aware of the idea that preschoolers’ self-directed private
speech during problem solving might serve as a bridge between active verbal support
from adults and completely silent thinking by the child. Instead teachers often
interpreted private speech as a sign of a child’s persistent immaturity rather than as a step toward
the greater maturity of silent thought. In class, therefore, they sometimes discouraged
children from talking to themselves, rather than allowing or respecting this
behavior. To deal with this misunderstanding, Deniz recommended professional
development education about language development.
Another
factor that limits adoption of cognitive developmental research is the extent of
cultural diversity and of associated differences in values and behaviors. Culturally
based interaction patterns that are comfortable to researchers or even to participants
in a study may be inappropriate outside the study context (Sternberg, 2007;
Tharp & Dalton, 2007), even including the contexts served by early
childhood programs. A frequent finding of child development research, for
example, is that lively give-and-take between child and adult is not only
beneficial cognitively, but also trainable in parents and children. Yet some
families—or even whole communities within society—may consider such interaction
disrespectful on the part of the child, and therefore discourage children from
engaging in it (Philips, 2009). Many cognitive developmental studies also
depend on children’s willingness to engage with problems or tasks out of their
normal intellectual or practical context, and at the behest of relatively
unknown adults. Judging by cross-cultural and other ethnographic research, however,
such interactions may pose problems that are social as well as cognitive. In observing
child care centers in Asia and the United States, for example, Tobin and his
associates found a range of practices at meal times, some of which seem less likely
to create extended table conversation with the qualities recommended earlier in
this chapter (Tobin, Hsueh, & Karasawa, 2009). At one center (in Japan), the
youngest children were served not by adults, but by older children. Mealtime conversation presumably did happen
during these encounters, but it is reasonable to suspect that their
conversation may have been more limited linguistically and/or more focused on the
immediate tasks of eating. In any case the setting was not really what American
educators and psychologists have in mind in recommending meal times as a venue
for developing cognitive skills (Snow and Beals 2006;Hamilton & Wilson,
2009; Kiser, et al., 2010). In another child care center (in the USA) observed
by Tobin, staff members were obliged by certification requirements to serve meals
“family style,” meaning that preschool children were not served by adults, but helped
themselves from bowls of food set at group tables. Again, developmentally productive
conversations may have happened anyway, but the need to monitor spills, excess
helpings, and logistics with a large number of children suggests that
conversations might easily become more focused on management than in the family
meal settings as described in research studies.
An
especially important factor limiting adoption of research on cognitive
development, however, stems from incommensurability between the goals and
conditions of research studies, compared to the goals and conditions typical of
early childhood classrooms. I alluded to some of these already in this chapter,
and they have been discussed thoroughly elsewhere in the literature on
educational research (e.g. Reason & Bradbury, 2006). Whereas developmental
psychology research aims for (and often achieves) control over confounding
factors, a major purpose of early childhood classroom practice is not to
control “extraneous” factors, but to recognize and work with them—i.e. work
with the entire context of a child’s experience. Whereas research aims for
conceptual clarity, early childhood practice aims for broad human development
and welfare. The differences mean that the factors that can make research
“fail” are the very ones that can make practice successful. The confounding
factors take many forms: the continual distraction and influence of peers, the personal
diversity among children, the continual revision of lesson plans on the fly, the
interplay of ideas, needs, and issues from persons outside the classroom. As
has often been pointed out, classrooms are therefore messy, but research
studies are comparatively clean, or at least try to be. As a result, it is hard
to “see” the results of research studies in many classroom situations. Of
particular relevance to this chapter, classroom tasks and activities that are
purely cognitive are rare or nonexistent; all cognitive activities either serve
additional social or emotional purposes, or else begin very explicitly as a
social encounter. Given that early childhood programs tend by definition to be group
settings, a pervasive social influence should not be surprising even if the
nominal focus of some activities is cognitive.
It
should also not surprise us that teachers, parents, and children construe the
idea of intelligence not as a
strictly cognitive talent, but as a quality that integrates social skills with
cognition. In interview studies by Sara Harkness and her associates, for
example, teachers from five societies (Italy, The Netherlands, Poland, Spain,
and USA) described their “best” or ideal students in terms that were at least
as social as cognitive (Harkness, et al., 2007). The best students were not
necessarily the cleverest ones intellectually, nor the highest performing on
cognitive tasks. Instead they were the ones who mixed lively personality,
creativity, and social and emotional sensitivity to others—qualities that
presumably facilitated their ability to learn from others. The teachers’ vocabulary
for describing these qualities varied, of course, but there was an underlying
sense that cognitive development did not happen in a social or emotional
vacuum, but was made possible by social interactions and relationships. Parents
in these societies echoed these attitudes. Even when some used the term smart to describe their own child (a tendency
especially of American parents), further questioning suggested that they were
talking about cognitive outcomes that were made possible by social skills
(Harkness & Super, 2006). Even young children themselves seem to regard
intelligence in similar terms. In an ethnographic study by Chen (2010), for
example, first-grade students described classmates as only mildly successful as students if they performed well academically,
but much more successful if they were socially popular. Academic
success, it seemed, was implicitly regarded as a component of social success,
not as a separate domain of it.
What
actually may be surprising is that political and funding agencies have not
recognized these beliefs and attitudes, but instead have promoted the teaching
of cognitive skills as if they exist independently of other domains of
development (No Child Left Behind Act, 2001; Lewin, 2010; see also Zigler &
Bishop-Josef, 2006). Partly as a result, assessments of early childhood
programs often make this same distinction (e.g. Burger, 2009; Camilli, et al.,
2010; Dee & Jacobson, 2010; Mashburn, et al., 2008); the social basis of
cognition is either are either ignored or assessed as if it develops independently
of cognition. The long-term result has been to pressure early childhood
educators to teach specific cognitive skill behaviors, but to leave them to
their own devices when teaching the social skills that make cognition possible,
such as ability to self-regulate attention or to learn by watching and
listening to others. The pressure, along with recent trends to limit public
budgets, has made it difficult for early childhood educators to implement the
best possible programs for children. As one program administrator put it, “we
are caught between the NCLB and the NAEYC”—between accountability for
instilling academic skills and commitment to developmentally appropriate
practice (Tobin, Hseuh, & Karasawa, 2009, p. 185ff). Even if educators can eventually
learn to live with this dilemma themselves, it will limit their effectiveness
with children indirectly.
But
all hope is not lost. A good starting point is the fact, noted above, that
teachers often already recognize the importance of social relationships and
interactions for developing children’s cognitive functions. One logical next
step may therefore be to follow Deniz’s advice mentioned above: to share the relevant
research as widely as possible early childhood educators—especially any research
that offers details of how relationships and interactions affect cognition. In
addition to such professional development, however, it would also help to
listen more closely to the comments and questions that early childhood
educators themselves consider most important for their work, and to adjust or
at least reinterpret the cognitive research agenda accordingly. Toward this
goal—and more radically—it would help to bridge the research-practice divide by
inviting developmental psychologists to take a turn walking in the shoes of practicing
early childhood educators. Doing so might vividly clarify to them how the
epistemology, the freedoms to act or lack thereof, and the priority questions
about children differ between the academic and professional worlds. Researchers
may or may not want to alter their research agendas in view of this additional knowledge,
and in fact may not always be able to so. But at least they would know more
clearly what questions educators care about the most, and they would be able to
assess more accurately the usefulness of existing developmental research in the
landscape of educational and societal needs.
References
Asamen,
J., Ellis, M., & Berry, G. (Eds.). (2008). The Sage Handbook of child development, multiculturalism, and media. Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage.
Barbarin,
O. & Wasik, B. (Eds.). (2009). Handbook
of child development and early education: Research to practice. New York: Guilford Press.
Bauer,
P. (2005). New developments in the study of infant memory. In D. M. Teti (Ed.),
Blackwell handbook of research methods in
developmental science, pp. 467-488. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
Bauer,
P. (2007). Remembering the times of our
lives: Memory in infancy and beyond. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Bauer,
P. (2009). Neurodevelopmental changes in infancy and beyond: Implications for
learning and memory. In Barbarin, O. & Wasik, B. (Eds.). (2009). Handbook of child development and early
education: Research to practice, pp. 78-102. New York: Guilford Press.
Berk,
L., Mann, T., & Ogan, A. Make-believe play: Wellspring for development of
self-regulation. In Singer, D., Golinkoff, R., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (Eds.).
(2006). Play=learning: How play motivates
and enhances children’s cognitive and social-emotional growth, pp. 74-100.
New York: Oxford University Press.
Boland,
A., Haden, C., & Ornstein, P. (2003). Boosting children’s memory by
training mothers in the use of an elaborative conversational style as an event
unfolds. Journal of Cognition and
Development, 4(1), 39-65.
Bradley,
B. & Reinking, D. (2010). Enhancing research and practice in early
childhood through formative and design experiments. Early child development and care, 180, 1-15.
Bredekamp,
S. & Copple, C. (Eds.). (1997). Developmentally
appropriate practice in early childhood programs (rev. ed.). Washington,
D.C.: National Association for the Education of Young Children.
Bruner,
J. (1960). The process of education.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Burger,
K. (2009). How does early childhood care and education affect cognitive
development? Early Childhood Research
Quarterly, 25, 140-165.
Callanan,
M., Siegel, D., & Luce, M. (2007). Conventionality in family conversations
about everyday objects. New Directions in
Child and Adolescent Development, #115, pp. 83-96.
Camilli,
G., Vargas, S., Ryan, S., & Barnett, S. (2010). Meta-analysis of the
effects of early education interventions on cognitive and social development. Teachers’ College Record, #112(3),
579-620.
Chen,
R. (2010). Early childhood identity:
Construction, culture, and the self. New York: Peter Lang.
Dee,
T. & Jacob, B. (2009). The impact of
No Child Left Behind on student achievement. Cambridge, MA: National Bureau
of Economic Research.
Deniz,
C. (2009). Early childhood teachers’ awareness, beliefs, and practices toward
children’s private speech. In Winsler, A., Fernyhough, C., & Montero, I.
(2009). Private speech, executive
functioning, and the development verbal self-regulation, pp. 236-246. New
York: Cambridge University Press.
Dick,
A. & Overton, W. (2010). Executive function: Description and explanation.
In Sokol, B., Muller, U., Carpendale, J., Young, A., & Iarocci, G. (Eds.).
(2010). Self and social regulation:
Social interaction and the development social understanding and executive
functions, pp. 2-34. New York: Oxford University Press.
Eilan,
N. & Heal, C., McCormack, T., & Roessler, J. (Eds.). (2005). Joint attention: Communication and other
minds. New York: Oxford University Press.
Fernyhough,
C. & Fradley, E. (2005). Private speech on an executive task: Relations
with task difficulty and task performance. Cognitive
Development, 20, 10-120.
Fitneva,
S. (2009). Evidentiality and trust: The effect of informational goals. New Directions in Child and Adolescent
Development, #123, pp. 49-62
Fusaro,
M. & Nelson, C. (2009). Developmental cognitive neuroscience and
educational practice. In O. Barbarin & B. Wasik (Eds.), Handbook of child development and early
education: Research to practice, pp. 55-77. New York: Guilford Press.
Garon,
N., Bryson, S., & Smith, I. (2008). Executive function in preschoolers: A
review using an integrative framework. Psychological
Bulletin, 134(1), 31-60.
Graham,
S., Stock, H., & Henderson, A. (2006).
Nineteen-month-olds’ understanding of the conventionality of object
labels versus desires. Infancy, 9, 341-350.
Haden,
C., Ornstein, P., Eckerman, C., & Didow, S. (2001). Mother-child
conversational interactions as events unfold: Linkages to subsequent
remembering. Child Development, 72, 1016-1031.
Haith,
M. & Benson, J. (2008). Encyclopedia
of infant and early childhood development. Boston: Elsevier/Academic Press.
Hala,
S., Pexman, P., Climie, E., Rostad, K., & Glenwright, M. (2010). A
bidirectional view of executive function and social interaction. In Sokol, B.,
Muller, U., Carpendale, J., Young, A., & Iarocci, G. (Eds.). (2010). Self and social regulation: Social
interaction and the development social understanding and executive functions, pp.
293-311. New York: Oxford University Press.
Hamilton,
S. & Wilson, J. (2009). Family mealtimes: Worth the effort? Infant, child, and adolescent nutrition,
1(6), 346-350.
Harkness,
S., Blom, M., Oliva, A., Moscardino, U., Zylicz, P., Bermudez, M., Feng, X.,
Carrasco-Zylicz, A., Axia, G., & Super, C. (2007). Teachers’ ethnotheories
of the ‘ideal student’ in five western cultures. Comparative Education, 43(1), 113-135.
Harkness,
S. & Super, C. (2006). Themes and variations: Parental ethnotheories in
western cultures. In K. Rubin (Ed.), Parental
beliefs, behaviors, and parent-child relations: A cross-cultural perspective,
pp. 61-80. New York: Psychology Press.
Helburn,
S. (1995). Cost, quality, and child
outcomes in childcare centers. Denver, CO: Center for Research in Economics
and Social Policy, University of Colorado.
Hendrick,
A., Haden, C., & Ornstein, P. (2009). Elaborative talking during and after an
event: Conversational style influences children’s remembering. Journal of Cognition and Development, 10(3),
188-209.
Hedrick,
A., San Souci, P., H. Gaden, C., & Ornstein, P. (2009). Mother-child joint
conversational exchanges during events: Linkages to children’s event memory
over time. Journal of Cognition and
Development, 10(3), 143-161.
Holmes,
R. (2010). “Do you like Doritos?”: Preschoolers’ table talk during lunchtime. Early child development and care, 180, 1-12.
Hrabok,
M., & Kerns, K. (2010). The development of self-regulation: A neuropsychological
perspective. In Sokol, B., Muller, U., Carpendale, J., Young, A., &
Iarocci, G. (Eds.). (2010). Self and
social regulation: Social interaction and the development social understanding
and executive functions, pp. 129-154. New York: Oxford University Press.
Jeschull,
L. & Roeper, T. (2009). Evidentiality vs. certainty: Do children trust
their minds more than their eyes? In Crawford, J., Otaki, K., & Takahashi,
M. (Eds.). Proceedings of the 3rd
conference on generative approaches to language acquisition North America, pp.
107-115. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project. Accessed from http://www.lingref.com on July 8, 2010.
Kalish,
C. (2005). Becoming status conscious: Children’s appreciation of social reality.
Philosophical explorations, 8(3), 245-263.
Kalish,
C. & Sabbagh, M. (2007). Conventionality and cognitive development:
Learning to think the right way. New
Directions in Child and Adolescent Development, #115, pp. 1-7.
Kagan,
J. & Herskowitz, N. (2005). A young
mind in a growing brain. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. In Barbarin, O. & Wasik,
B. (Eds.). (2009). Handbook of child
development and early education: Research to practice, pp. 579-597. New
York: Guilford Press.
Kiser,
L., Medoff, D., Nurse, W., Black, M., & Fiese, B. (2010). Family mealtime
Q-sort: A measure of mealtime practices. Journal
of Family Psychology, 24(1), 92-96.
Koenig,
M. & Harris, P. (2005). Preschoolers’ mistrust of ignorant and inaccurate
speakers. Child Development, 6, 1261-1277.
Kuhn,
D. & Siegler, R. (Eds.). (2006). Handbook
of child psychology, Volume 2: Cognition, perception, and language.
Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Landry,
S., Miller-Loncar, C., Smith, K., & Swank, P. (2002). The role of early
parenting in children’s development of executive processes. Developmental Neuropsychology, 21, 15-41.
Lewin,
T. (2010, July 21). “Many states adopt national standards for their schools.” New York Times, p. A1.
Lewis,
C. & Carpendale, J. (2009). Introduction: Links between social interaction
and executive function. In C. Lewis & J. Carpendale (Eds.), Social
interaction and development of executive function. New Directions in Child and Adolescent Development, 123, 1-15.
Linn,
S. (2008). The case for make-believe
play: Saving play in a commercialized world. New York: The New Press.
Marti,
E. (2003). Strengths and weaknesses of cognition over preschool years. In J.
Valsiner & K. Connolly (Eds.), Sage
handbook of developmental psychology, pp. 257-275. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Martin,
J. & Failows, L. (2010). Executive function: Theoretical concerns. In Sokol, B., Muller, U., Carpendale,
J., Young, A., & Iarocci, G. (Eds.). (2010). Self and social regulation: Social interaction and the development
social understanding and executive functions, pp. 35-55. New York: Oxford
University Press.
Mashburn,
A., Pianta, R., Hamre, B., Downer, J., Barbarin, O., Bryant, D., Burchinal, M.,
Early, D. & Howes, C. (2008). Measures of classroom quality in
prekindergarten and children’s development academic, language and social skills.
Child Development, 79(3), 732-749.
Matsui,
T. & Fitneva, S. (2009). Knowing how we know: Evidentiality and cognitive
development. New Directions in Child and
Adolescent Development, #123, pp. 1-11.
Muller,
U., Jacques, S., Brocki, K., & Zelazo, P. (2009). The executive functions
of language in preschool children. In Winsler, A., Fernyhough, C., &
Montero, I. (2009). Private speech,
executive functioning, and the development verbal self-regulation, pp.
53-68. New York: Cambridge University Press.
National
Association for the Education of Young Children. (2009). Position statement on developmentally appropriate practice in early
childhood programs. Washington, D.C.: Author. Accessed on June 29, 2010 at http://www.naeyc.org/files/naeyc/file/positions/position%20statement%20Web.pdf.
Odom,
S., Barbarin, O., & Wasik, B. (2009). Applying lessons from developmental
science to early education. In Barbarin, O. & Wasik, B. (Eds.). Handbook of child development and early
education: Research to practice, pp. 579-597. New York: Guilford Press.
Ornstein,
P., Haden, C., & Hedrick, A. (2004). Learning to remember:
Social-communicative exchanges and the development of children’s memory skills.
Developmental Review, 24, 374-395.
Piaget,
J. (1962). Plays, dreams, and imitation
in childhood. New York: Norton.
Pexman,
P. (2008). It’s fascinating research: The cognition of verbal irony. Current directions in psychological science,
17, 286-290.
Pexman,
P., Zdrazilova, L., McConnachie, D., Deater-Deckard, K., & Petrill, S.
(2009). “That was smooth, Mom”: Children’s production of verbal and gestural
irony. Metaphor and symbol, 24(4),
237-248.
Pexman,
P., Whalen, J., & Green, J. (2010). Understanding verbal irony: Clues from
interpretation of direct and indirect ironic remarks. Discourse processes, 47, 237-261.
Philips,
S. (2009). Participant structures and communicative competence: The Warm
Springs children in community and classroom. In A. Duranti (Ed.), Linguistic anthropology: A reader, 2nd
edition, pp. 329-344. Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell.
Rakoczy,
H. (2007). Play, games, and the development of collective intentionality. New directions in child and adolescent
development, #115, pp. 53-67.
Reason,
P. & Bradbury, H. (Eds.). (2006). Handbook
of action research. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Ritchie,
S., Maxwell, K., & Bredekamp, S. (2010). Rethinking early schooling: Using
developmental science to transform children’s early school experiences. In
Barbarin, O. & Wasik, B. (Eds.). (2009). Handbook of child development and early education: Research to
practice, pp. 14-37. New York: Guilford Press.
Sabbagh,
M. & Baldwin, D. (2005). Understanding the role of communicative intentions
in word learning. In Eilan, N. & Heal, C., McCormack, T., & Roessler,
J. (Eds.). (2005). Joint attention:
Communication and other minds, pp. 165-184. New York: Oxford University
Press.
Sabbagh,
M. & Henderson, A. (2007). How an appreciation of conventionality shapes
early word learning. New Directions in
Child and Adolescent Development, #115.
Seifert,
K. (2006). Cognitive development and the education of young children. In B.
Spodek & O. Saracho (Eds.). Handbook
of research on the education of young children, pp. 9-22.
Singer,
D., Golinkoff, R., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (Eds.). (2006). Play=learning: How play motivates and enhances children’s cognitive and
social-emotional growth. New York: Oxford University Press.
Sokol,
B., Muller, U., Carpendale, J., Young, A., & Iarocci, G. (Eds.). (2010). Self and social regulation: Social
interaction and the development social understanding and executive functions.
New York: Oxford University Press.
Snow,
C. & Beals, D. (2006). Mealtime talk that supports literacy development. New Directions in Child and Adolescent
Development, #111, 51-66.
Sternberg,
R. (2007). Culture, instruction, and assessment. Comparative Education, 43(1), 5-22.
Stetsenko,
A. & Vianna, E. (2009). Bridging developmental theory and educational
practice. In Barbarin, O. & Wasik, B. (Eds.). (2009). Handbook of child development and early education: Research to
practice, pp. 38-54. New York: Guilford Press.
Tharp,
R. & Dalton, S. (2007). Orthodoxy, cultural compatibility, and universals
in education. Comparative Education, 43(1),
53-79.
Tobin,
J., Hseuh, Y., & Karasawa, M. (2009). Preschool
in three cultures revisited. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Wellman,
H. (2003). Understanding the psychological world: Developing a theory of mind.
In U. Goswani (Ed.), Blackwell handbook
of childhood cognitive development, pp. 167-187. Malden, MA: Blackwell
Publishers.
Wells,
G. (2009). The social context of language and literacy development. In
Barbarin, O. & Wasik, B. (Eds.). (2009). Handbook of child development and early education: Research to
practice, pp. 271-302. New York: Guilford Press.
Winsler,
A., Fernyhough, C., & Montero, I. (2009). Private speech, executive functioning, and the development verbal
self-regulation. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Woodward,
A. (2005). Infants’ joint attention of the actions involved in joint attention.
In N. Eilan, N. & Heal, C., McCormack, T., & Roessler, J. (Eds.).
(2005). Joint attention: Communication
and other minds, pp. 110-128. New York: Oxford University Press.
Zigler,
E. & Bishop-Josef, S. (2006). The cognitive child versus the whole child:
Lessons from 40 years of Head Start. In Singer, D., Golinkoff, R., &
Hirsh-Pasek, K. (Eds.). (2006). Play=learning:
How play motivates and enhances children’s cognitive and social-emotional growth,
pp. 15-35. New York: Oxford University Press.