Here are some of the major differences between the vowels of western Canadian English and the cardinal vowels whose symbols we have been using to right them.
We can see this in formant plots:
For many younger speakers, the high central rounded vowel symbol [] would be more
accurate. Some extreme versions, like exaggerated imitations of
California dude, are almost as front as the front rounded [y] of
French lune 'moon'.
You might have to deliberately pull your tongue body backwards in
order to produce a cardinal [u]. The closest most Canadian English
speakers will get in natural speech is when the [u] comes before a dark
[], as in
school. Anticipating the velarization on the [
] helps keep the [u]
in a back, velar position. You should be able to hear the difference
between the almost cardinal [u] in school [sku
] and the quite front
[u] in cute [kj
t].
school |
![]() |
cute | ![]() |
(The backing effect of [] is especially noticeable when it doesn't work. Many
younger speakers can pronounce slang cool (or kewl) with the
/u/ as far front as it would normally be for cute.)
Even in accents of Canadian English that use monophthongs for bait and boat, the monophthongs will typically be lower than cardinal [e] and [o].
This feature of British English is also found in many other dialects
of English. Even in many dialects of North America, the beginning of the
/o/ diphthong is becoming increasingly centralized and increasingly
unrounded.
![]() |
'grandson' |
![]() |
'thief' |
Nor do diphthongs always have to move upward and to the front or back. It is particularly common for languages to have "centring" diphthongs, where the off-glide is schwa rather than [j] or [w].
In English, some parts of the U.S. use [] or [e
] for
/æ/, bad [be
d]. Centring diphthongs are also used widely in
"non-rhotic" dialects -- the loss of historical [
] from syllable codas does
not necessarily result in homophones:
bead | ![]() |
bid | ![]() |
beard | ![]() |